Attention Cost of Unfair Treatment (JMP)
Attention Cost of Unfair Treatment (JMP)
Unfair treatment, whether actual or perceived, is widespread in the labor market. Using an online experiment, I study the reduction in effort through the channel of being cognitively distracted due to unfair treatment. This decreased focus and attention affects work-related activities and leads to worse financial outcomes. The literature on discrimination has mainly focused on the direct effects, like blocked opportunities or job satisfaction, but less on the indirect effects, like worsening work performance. Online participants from Prolific engage in a real-effort task with a minimum effort requirement to earn a financial bonus. Subjects recruited in the first stage of the experiment decide whether to perform a real-effort task with a minimum effort provision or take the financial bonus from another subject and skip doing the work task. Subsequent subjects then perform the real-effort task, learn whether the financial bonus was taken, and complete two attention checks, a CRT questionnaire, and a Raven's test. The control group perform the task without payment and without the chance of losing the bonus.
Unfair treatment, whether actual or perceived, is widespread in the labor market. Using an online experiment, I study the reduction in effort through the channel of being cognitively distracted due to unfair treatment. This decreased focus and attention affects work-related activities and leads to worse financial outcomes. The literature on discrimination has mainly focused on the direct effects, like blocked opportunities or job satisfaction, but less on the indirect effects, like worsening work performance. Online participants from Prolific engage in a real-effort task with a minimum effort requirement to earn a financial bonus. Subjects recruited in the first stage of the experiment decide whether to perform a real-effort task with a minimum effort provision or take the financial bonus from another subject and skip doing the work task. Subsequent subjects then perform the real-effort task, learn whether the financial bonus was taken, and complete two attention checks, a CRT questionnaire, and a Raven's test. The control group perform the task without payment and without the chance of losing the bonus.
with O. Giuntella & J. Costa-Font (submitted)
with O. Giuntella & J. Costa-Font (submitted)
We study the effect of the 1998 UK Working Time Regulation (WTR 98) on the use of time and the well-being of employees. Using data from the British Household Panel Survey and an event-study analysis approach, we compare trends in the outcomes of workers exposed to the regulation before and after the reform. We find that workers working long hours before the reform reduced their hours and reallocated time towards sleep, leisure, and related personal care activities. Exposed workers were less likely to report dissatisfaction with their jobs, and we find some evidence of an increase in happiness and in the likelihood of reporting no anxiety or depression among women. We do not observe any significant income change or a notable impact on satisfaction with earnings. Instead, there is evidence of an increase in the employment and earnings of the partners of exposed workers, driven by women.
We study the effect of the 1998 UK Working Time Regulation (WTR 98) on the use of time and the well-being of employees. Using data from the British Household Panel Survey and an event-study analysis approach, we compare trends in the outcomes of workers exposed to the regulation before and after the reform. We find that workers working long hours before the reform reduced their hours and reallocated time towards sleep, leisure, and related personal care activities. Exposed workers were less likely to report dissatisfaction with their jobs, and we find some evidence of an increase in happiness and in the likelihood of reporting no anxiety or depression among women. We do not observe any significant income change or a notable impact on satisfaction with earnings. Instead, there is evidence of an increase in the employment and earnings of the partners of exposed workers, driven by women.
with V. Burdea & J. Woon
with V. Burdea & J. Woon
We investigate how different belief elicitation methods influence reported prior beliefs about induced (objectively described) probabilities in the online environment. We compare two incentive compatible methods, the binarized scoring rule (BSR) and the stochastic Becker-deGroot-Marschak mechanism (BDM), with unincentivized introspection (INTRO). We find that subjects perceive the incentive compatible methods as significantly more difficult. Moreover, the error in priors is significantly higher for the incentivized methods than for INTRO. Lower perceived difficulty and higher probabilistic skills do not reduce the error in priors between the incentive compatible methods and INTRO. With learning, however, both the BSR and BDM methods lead to higher errors in priors. Higher numeracy significantly and almost perfectly compensates for the increased error (reducing the error by an approximately equal amount) in the BDM condition, but not in the BSR condition.
We investigate how different belief elicitation methods influence reported prior beliefs about induced (objectively described) probabilities in the online environment. We compare two incentive compatible methods, the binarized scoring rule (BSR) and the stochastic Becker-deGroot-Marschak mechanism (BDM), with unincentivized introspection (INTRO). We find that subjects perceive the incentive compatible methods as significantly more difficult. Moreover, the error in priors is significantly higher for the incentivized methods than for INTRO. Lower perceived difficulty and higher probabilistic skills do not reduce the error in priors between the incentive compatible methods and INTRO. With learning, however, both the BSR and BDM methods lead to higher errors in priors. Higher numeracy significantly and almost perfectly compensates for the increased error (reducing the error by an approximately equal amount) in the BDM condition, but not in the BSR condition.
In this chapter, we review the evidence on the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on health behaviors and explore observational data on mental health, anxiety medications, and time use. We focus on the most vulnerable populations: young adults, parents and children, essential workers, and minorities. First, we explore the heterogeneity in the impact of COVID-19 on mental health, and then examine more closely the impact on time use with a specific focus on health behaviors (exercise, sleeping, eating habits) and personal care.
In this chapter, we review the evidence on the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on health behaviors and explore observational data on mental health, anxiety medications, and time use. We focus on the most vulnerable populations: young adults, parents and children, essential workers, and minorities. First, we explore the heterogeneity in the impact of COVID-19 on mental health, and then examine more closely the impact on time use with a specific focus on health behaviors (exercise, sleeping, eating habits) and personal care.
Behavioral economics is an increasingly influential field across the social sciences, including public administration. But while some behavioral economics ideas have spread rapidly in public administration research, we argue that a broader range of behavioral economics concepts can and should be applied. We begin by outlining some central models and concepts from behavioral economics to fix ideas, including the rational model and the “behavioral” response. We then discuss how a variety of heretofore underutilized behavioral economics concepts can be applied to a specific area of work in public administration – bureaucratic decision making. Our aim in doing so is two-fold. First, we hope to provide fresh food for thought for researchers and practitioners working in the broader behavioral public administration space. Second, we hope to demonstrate that there is substantial scope for expanding behavioral economics’ influence on public administration research.
Behavioral economics is an increasingly influential field across the social sciences, including public administration. But while some behavioral economics ideas have spread rapidly in public administration research, we argue that a broader range of behavioral economics concepts can and should be applied. We begin by outlining some central models and concepts from behavioral economics to fix ideas, including the rational model and the “behavioral” response. We then discuss how a variety of heretofore underutilized behavioral economics concepts can be applied to a specific area of work in public administration – bureaucratic decision making. Our aim in doing so is two-fold. First, we hope to provide fresh food for thought for researchers and practitioners working in the broader behavioral public administration space. Second, we hope to demonstrate that there is substantial scope for expanding behavioral economics’ influence on public administration research.
According to the World Health Organization (WHO, 2022), the COVID-19 pandemic triggered a 25% increase in the prevalence of anxiety and depression worldwide. Social isolation, loneliness, fear of infection, bereavement and financial concerns have contributed to the observed rise in anxiety and depression. The pandemic, the necessary public health interventions, and the negative impact on economic activity had long-lasting effects on people’s mental health. This chapter reviews the literature on COVID-19 and mental wellbeing. It first examines the role of public health measures, school lockdowns, economic uncertainty, and changes in working arrangements, as well as the effects of vaccine rollout. The second part of the chapter explores the heterogeneity of the COVID-19 impact in the population, documenting alarming tends in the mental health of the most vulnerable populations such as young adults, women, families with children, minorities, and essential workers.
According to the World Health Organization (WHO, 2022), the COVID-19 pandemic triggered a 25% increase in the prevalence of anxiety and depression worldwide. Social isolation, loneliness, fear of infection, bereavement and financial concerns have contributed to the observed rise in anxiety and depression. The pandemic, the necessary public health interventions, and the negative impact on economic activity had long-lasting effects on people’s mental health. This chapter reviews the literature on COVID-19 and mental wellbeing. It first examines the role of public health measures, school lockdowns, economic uncertainty, and changes in working arrangements, as well as the effects of vaccine rollout. The second part of the chapter explores the heterogeneity of the COVID-19 impact in the population, documenting alarming tends in the mental health of the most vulnerable populations such as young adults, women, families with children, minorities, and essential workers.
In Philadelphia, an estimated 1 billion single-use disposable plastic bags are used annually, contributing to carbon emissions, plastic waste, and litter. In response to these environmental impacts, the Philadelphia City Council passed Bill 190610 in 2019, which banned retail establishments from distributing single-use plastic bags and paper bags not made of at least 40% recycled material. To assess the effectiveness of the ban, the research team compared plastic bag usage in grocery stores in Philadelphia (“Philadelphia” sample) with usage in surrounding suburbs (“Suburbs” sample). The results of the study showed a significant decrease in plastic bag usage in the city after the ban was implemented. Extrapolating the results from our sample, we estimate that the ban led to the elimination of over 200 million plastic bags in the city. This is roughly equivalent to filling Philadelphia City Hall with plastic bags every eight months.
In Philadelphia, an estimated 1 billion single-use disposable plastic bags are used annually, contributing to carbon emissions, plastic waste, and litter. In response to these environmental impacts, the Philadelphia City Council passed Bill 190610 in 2019, which banned retail establishments from distributing single-use plastic bags and paper bags not made of at least 40% recycled material. To assess the effectiveness of the ban, the research team compared plastic bag usage in grocery stores in Philadelphia (“Philadelphia” sample) with usage in surrounding suburbs (“Suburbs” sample). The results of the study showed a significant decrease in plastic bag usage in the city after the ban was implemented. Extrapolating the results from our sample, we estimate that the ban led to the elimination of over 200 million plastic bags in the city. This is roughly equivalent to filling Philadelphia City Hall with plastic bags every eight months.
Little is known about the extent to which financial institutions furnish information on natural disaster assistance or how furnishing may vary by industry and consumer type. This report documents the prevalence of natural disaster comment codes in credit records to shed light on current practices for natural disaster reporting. It also documents how this reporting may vary based on account characteristics and consumer credit score.
Little is known about the extent to which financial institutions furnish information on natural disaster assistance or how furnishing may vary by industry and consumer type. This report documents the prevalence of natural disaster comment codes in credit records to shed light on current practices for natural disaster reporting. It also documents how this reporting may vary based on account characteristics and consumer credit score.